+ All documents
Home > Documents > Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptation

Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptation

Date post: 11-Nov-2023
Category:
Upload: independent
View: 0 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend
15
J Physiol 591.4 (2013) pp 1081–1095 1081 The Journal of Physiology Neuroscience Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking tracks motor adaptation James M. Finley 1,2 , Amy J. Bastian 1,2 and Jinger S. Gottschall 3 1 Motion Analysis Laboratory, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA 2 Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA 3 Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA Key points Neuroscientists often suggest that we adapt our movements to minimize energy use; however, recent studies have provided conflicting evidence in this regard. In the present study, we show that motor learning robustly increases the economy of locomotion during split-belt treadmill adaptation. We also demonstrate that reductions in metabolic power scale with the magnitude of adaptation and are also associated with a reduction in muscle activity throughout the lower limbs. Our results provide strong evidence that increasing economy may be a key criterion driving the systematic changes in co-ordination during locomotor adaptation. These findings may also facilitate the design of novel interventions to improve locomotor learning in stroke survivors. Abstract Many theories of motor control suggest that we select our movements to reduce energy use. However, it is unclear whether this process underlies short-term motor adaptation to novel environments. Here we asked whether adaptation to walking on a split-belt treadmill leads to a more economical walking pattern. We hypothesized that adaptation would be accompanied by a reduction in metabolic power and muscle activity and that these reductions would be temporally correlated. Eleven individuals performed a split-belt adaptation task where the belt speeds were set at 0.5 and 1.5 m s 1 . Adaptation was characterized by step length symmetry, which is the normalized difference in step length between the legs. Metabolic power was calculated based on expired gas analysis, and surface EMG was used to record the activity of four bilateral leg muscles (tibialis anterior, lateral gastrocnemius, vastus lateralis and biceps femoris). All participants initially walked with unequal step lengths when the belts moved at different speeds, but gradually adapted to take steps of equal length. Additionally, net metabolic power was reduced from early adaptation to late adaptation (early, 3.78 ± 1.05 W kg 1 ; and late, 3.05 ± 0.79 W kg 1 ; P < 0.001). This reduction in power was also accompanied by a bilateral reduction in EMG throughout the gait cycle. Furthermore, the reductions in metabolic power occurred over the same time scale as the improvements in step length symmetry, and the magnitude of these improvements predicted the size of the reduction in metabolic power. Our results suggest that increasing economy may be a key criterion driving locomotor adaptation. (Received 25 September 2012; accepted after revision 14 December 2012; first published online 17 December 2012) Corresponding author J. M. Finley: Kennedy Krieger Institute, Motion Analysis Laboratory, Room G-04, 707 North Broadway, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA. Email: j-fi[email protected] A.J.B. and J.S.G. contributed equally to this work. C 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C 2013 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245506
Transcript

J Physiol 591.4 (2013) pp 1081–1095 1081

The

Jou

rnal

of

Phys

iolo

gy

Neuroscience Learning to be economical: the energy cost of walking

tracks motor adaptation

James M. Finley1,2, Amy J. Bastian1,2 and Jinger S. Gottschall3

1Motion Analysis Laboratory, Kennedy Krieger Institute, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA2Department of Neuroscience, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA3Department of Kinesiology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, USA

Key points

• Neuroscientists often suggest that we adapt our movements to minimize energy use; however,recent studies have provided conflicting evidence in this regard.

• In the present study, we show that motor learning robustly increases the economy of locomotionduring split-belt treadmill adaptation.

• We also demonstrate that reductions in metabolic power scale with the magnitude of adaptationand are also associated with a reduction in muscle activity throughout the lower limbs.

• Our results provide strong evidence that increasing economy may be a key criterion drivingthe systematic changes in co-ordination during locomotor adaptation.

• These findings may also facilitate the design of novel interventions to improve locomotorlearning in stroke survivors.

Abstract Many theories of motor control suggest that we select our movements to reduce energyuse. However, it is unclear whether this process underlies short-term motor adaptation to novelenvironments. Here we asked whether adaptation to walking on a split-belt treadmill leads to amore economical walking pattern. We hypothesized that adaptation would be accompanied by areduction in metabolic power and muscle activity and that these reductions would be temporallycorrelated. Eleven individuals performed a split-belt adaptation task where the belt speeds wereset at 0.5 and 1.5 m s−1. Adaptation was characterized by step length symmetry, which is thenormalized difference in step length between the legs. Metabolic power was calculated based onexpired gas analysis, and surface EMG was used to record the activity of four bilateral leg muscles(tibialis anterior, lateral gastrocnemius, vastus lateralis and biceps femoris). All participantsinitially walked with unequal step lengths when the belts moved at different speeds, but graduallyadapted to take steps of equal length. Additionally, net metabolic power was reduced from earlyadaptation to late adaptation (early, 3.78 ± 1.05 W kg−1; and late, 3.05 ± 0.79 W kg−1; P < 0.001).This reduction in power was also accompanied by a bilateral reduction in EMG throughout thegait cycle. Furthermore, the reductions in metabolic power occurred over the same time scale asthe improvements in step length symmetry, and the magnitude of these improvements predictedthe size of the reduction in metabolic power. Our results suggest that increasing economy may bea key criterion driving locomotor adaptation.

(Received 25 September 2012; accepted after revision 14 December 2012; first published online 17 December 2012)Corresponding author J. M. Finley: Kennedy Krieger Institute, Motion Analysis Laboratory, Room G-04, 707 NorthBroadway, Baltimore, MD 21205, USA. Email: [email protected]

A.J.B. and J.S.G. contributed equally to this work.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society DOI: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.245506

1082 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

Abbreviations BF, biceps femoris; EMG, electromyography or electromyogram; LG, lateral gastrocnemius; MBA,maximal baseline activity; MIBA, maximal integrated baseline activity; SL, step length; SS, step length symmetry; STS,step time symmetry; TA, tibialis anterior; t f , fast step time; t s, slow step time; VL, vastus lateralis.

Introduction

Human walking is often highly stereotyped. Our legsoscillate in a reciprocal relationship, and we walkwith symmetric step lengths (Reisman et al. 2005)and step times. When the regularity associated withwalking is disturbed, we often adapt our movementsgradually to restore certain kinematic features throughan error-based learning process (Reisman et al. 2005;Emken & Reinkensmeyer, 2005; Lam et al. 2006). Forexample, an asymmetry between right and left steplengths can be introduced on a split-belt treadmillby moving each belt at a different speed. However,individuals gradually adapt their walking pattern toreduce this asymmetry and produce symmetric steplengths (Reisman et al. 2005, 2007; Choi et al. 2009).During normal walking, changes in step length can eitherincrease or reduce the economy of walking (Donelanet al. 2002; Umberger & Martin, 2007), defined as themetabolic power required to walk at a given speed, andtherefore one might expect that changes in economy occurduring split-belt adaptation.

Why does the nervous system adapt to take symmetricstep lengths in an asymmetric environment? Symmetry isby no means obligatory. People are capable of maintainingasymmetric step lengths given appropriate feedback(Malone & Bastian, 2010) or when the cerebellumis damaged (Morton & Bastian, 2006). Additionally,adapting to symmetric step lengths requires that otherparameters, such as the time between foot strikes, becomeincreasingly asymmetric (Malone et al. 2012). Manytheories of motor control suggest that movements arerefined so as to minimize energetic cost (Zarrugh et al.1974; Hatze & Buys, 1977; Alexander, 1997; Todorov, 2004;Emken et al. 2007). This is an intuitive goal, becauseeconomical movements enable us to use more energyto perform other tasks relevant to survival (Alexander,2002); therefore, it is possible that the adaptationpatterns observed during split-belt walking are drivenby a process that maximizes economy.

There is evidence that many elements of our locomotorpattern are selected to maximize economy during normalwalking. Economy is typically characterized by measuringthe rate of energy use, or metabolic power, which iscomputed from rates of oxygen consumption and carbondioxide production (Brockway, 1987). For a given speed,we select a step rate (Zarrugh & Radcliffe, 1978; Bertram& Ruina, 2001; Kuo, 2001; Umberger & Martin, 2007) andstep width (Donelan et al. 2001) that minimize metabolicpower. Thus, the walking pattern developed through yearsof practice appears to be the most economical for tasks that

we commonly experience. However, our walking patternmust also be flexible to adapt to novel environments.

What remains to be seen is whether short-termadaptation to novel locomotor tasks is consistent with astrategy to maximize economy. Motor adaptation occursthrough trial-and-error practice when a well-learnedmotor skill is performed in the presence of a novel,perturbing context or environment (Martin et al. 1996).Therefore, although people gradually increase economywhen learning a novel skill over weeks to months (Childset al. 2002; Lay et al. 2002; Galna & Sparrow, 2006; Sawicki& Ferris, 2008), it is still unknown whether economy ismaximized during locomotor adaptation which occurs ona much shorter time scale of minutes.

Here we asked whether adaptation to a split-belttreadmill leads to a more economical walking pattern. Inthis context, economy refers to the metabolic power usedto walk at a specific combination of belt speeds. It hasbeen demonstrated that individuals with asymmetric gaitsare less economical than able-bodied individuals (Waters& Mulroy, 1999); thus, it is possible that energy mini-mization drives the restoration of step length symmetryobserved during split-belt adaptation. Alternatively, theasymmetry in step timing developed over the courseof adaptation may result in a higher metabolic poweras adaptation progresses. We hypothesized that bothmetabolic power and muscle activity would decreaseover the course of adaptation, consistent with the ideathat the motor adaptation is driven in part by energyminimization.

Methods

Ethical approval

Eleven individuals participated in this study (sixmen, 22 ± 2 years old). The experimental protocol wasapproved by the The Pennsylvania State UniversityInstitutional Review Board and conformed to thestandards set by the Declaration of Helsinki. All participantsprovided written informed consent before testing.

Protocol

The participants learned to walk on a custom-builtsplit-belt treadmill capable of operating at independentspeeds for the left and right legs. This treadmill was similarto previously described devices (Kram et al. 1998; Collinset al. 2009) and included two, 4.5 kW variable speed,alternating current electronic motors. The treadmill belts

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1083

were controlled manually and accelerated from rest tothe maximal speed used in our experiments in less than0.5 s for all participants. Throughout the experiment, theparticipants walked while maintaining light hand contactwith rails on each side of the treadmill to provide stabilitywhen the belt speeds changed. The overall study protocolis illustrated in Fig. 1. The experiment began with a 10 minwarm-up period, with both belts matched at 1.0 m s−1. Forthe transition between each phase of the experiment, thebelt speeds were abruptly changed to the new speeds by oneof the experimenters. The warm-up was followed by twobaseline periods, during which participants walked withthe belts matched at 0.5 or 1.5 m s−1. The order of thesebaseline periods was randomized for each participant tominimize possible order effects on the metabolic powermeasured during the adaptation period. Upon completionof baseline walking, the participants walked for 12 minwith the left belt moving at 1.5 m s−1 while the right beltmoved at 0.5 m s−1 (3:1 ratio). These parameters wereselected to be consistent with previous studies of split-beltadaptation (Reisman et al. 2005; Malone & Bastian, 2010).

Data collection

Expired gas analysis. As the participants walked on thetreadmill, the rate of oxygen consumption and carbondioxide production was measured using a TrueOne

2400 metabolic measurement system (Parvomedics,Sandy, UT, USA). Before data collection, the systemwas allowed to warm up for 30 min to heat thepneumotachometer. After the warm-up period, the gasanalyser and pneumotachometer were calibrated to themanufacturer’s specifications. Expired gas was sampledby the sensors on a breath-by-breath basis, and the ratesof oxygen consumption and carbon dioxide productionwere computed. Metabolic data were collected for 5 minduring quiet standing and subtracted from the metabolicmeasurements made during all subsequent walking peri-ods to yield net metabolic rate.

Kinematics. Kinematic data were acquired with a digitalcamera system (Motion Analysis 3D Eagle, Santa Rosa,

CA, USA). Prior to data collection, retroreflective markerswere placed on the following anatomical landmarksbilaterally: anterior superior iliac spine; lateral femoralepicondyle; lateral malleolus; posterior calcaneous; andfirst metatarsal. The motion-analysis system consistedof six digital cameras connected through an Ethernethub to the data-collection computer. Kinematic datawere sampled at 100 Hz. Data were collected with EvaRT(version 3.21; Motion Analysis Corporation, Santa Rosa,CA, USA) for reduction and processing. Before eachdata-collection session, the motion-analysis system wascalibrated to the manufacturer’s recommendations.

Electromyography. Electromyographic signals weremeasured using a telemetered amplifier system (Bortec,Calgary, CA, USA). Prior to electrode placement, the skinwas prepared with fine sandpaper and alcohol. Bipolar,silver–silver chloride, surface electrodes (1-cm-diameterdiscs, 2 cm interelectrode distance; Vermed, Bellows Falls,VT, USA) were placed over the following four musclesbilaterally: tibialis anterior (TA); lateral gastrocnemius(LG); vastus lateralis (VL); and biceps femoris (BF),according to the recommendations by Cram & Kasman(1998). For the TA, LG and BF muscles, electrodes wereplaced over the approximate centre of the muscle belly. ForVL, the electrodes were placed over the distal third of themuscle lateral to the rectus femoris. The EMG amplifiergain was set to 2000, and the EMG signals were sampled at1000 Hz. We verified that the cross-talk between muscleswas negligible with a series of contractions suggested byWinter et al. (1994) and Cram & Kasman (1998).

Data analysis

Adaptation parameters. We calculated kinematicparameters associated with locomotor adaptation usingthree-dimensional marker positions. We evaluatedchanges in step length and step time because each hasbeen determined to make significant contributions to theeconomy of walking (Kuo, 2001; Donelan et al. 2002; Dokeet al. 2005). The onset of stance and swing were estimatedby computing the peak anterior and posterior limb angle

Warm-up

Belt Speed (m/s)

Post-AdaptationAdaptationBase 2Base 1

0.5

1.51.0

10 37322015Time (min)

Figure 1. Experimental protocolThe experiment began with a 10 min warm-up period at 1.0 m s−1 to minimize the transient changes in metabolicpower across each block. This was followed by two baseline periods at 0.5 and 1.5 m s−1, which were presentedin a random order for each participant. Baseline walking was followed by an adaptation period, during whichparticipants walked for 12 min with the left belt moving at 1.5 m s−1 and the right belt at 0.5 m s−1. Adaptationwas followed by a 5 min postadaptation period, during which both belts moved at 0.5 m s−1.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1084 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

excursions for each step, respectively. For this study, steplength symmetry was used as the primary measure ofadaptation, because prior studies have demonstrated thathealthy individuals adapt this parameter robustly acrossmultiple speed ratios (Reisman et al. 2005, 2007; Malone& Bastian, 2010). Step length symmetry (SS) is definedas the normalized difference between the step lengths ofeach limb, as follows [eqn (1)]:

SS = SLfast − SLslow

SLfast + SLslow(1)

Step length (SL) was defined as the anterior–posteriordistance between the markers on the lateral malleolus ofeach leg at heel strike. Fast step length was measured atheel strike of the limb on the fast belt and slow step lengthwas measured in a similar manner at heel strike of the slowlimb. Positive values for step length symmetry indicate thatthe fast step is larger than the slow step, and the converseholds for negative values. A step length symmetry value ofzero indicates that the fast and slow steps are of equal lengthand thus represents symmetry. A representative exampleof these parameters for a single participant illustrates howstep length and step length symmetry vary during theadaptation and postadaptation periods (Fig. 2A and B).

Temporal changes in the walking pattern were assessedusing measures of step timing. Step time was defined as thetime between consecutive heel strikes, where the slow steptime (t s) refers to the time between a heel strike on the slowbelt and the following heel strike on the fast belt. Fast steptime (t f ) was likewise defined as the time between a heelstrike on the fast belt and the subsequent heel strike onthe slow belt. This measure is equivalent to the reciprocalof cadence computed on a step-by-step basis. Step timesymmetry (STS) was defined as the normalized differencein step times across limbs as described in eqn (2):

STS = ts − tf

Tstride= ts − tf

ts + tf(2)

Here, T stride is the time between two consecutive heelstrikes on one limb, also known as the stride time. Astep time symmetry of zero indicates that the fast andslow step times are equal. A representative example from asingle participant demonstrates how individual step timesand step time symmetry vary throughout the course ofadaptation and postadaptation (Fig. 2C and D).

In all figures showing time series data, each of ourkinematic parameters was averaged within consecutive10 s bins. This facilitated comparison between thetemporal changes in kinematics and the temporal changesin metabolic power. For the group analysis, averagekinematic parameters were computed for the followingperiods: warm-up; slow baseline; fast baseline; earlyadaptation; late adaptation; early postadaptation; and late

postadaptation. For the warm-up and baseline periods,the average step length and step time parameters for eachparticipant were computed by averaging over all stridesrecorded during each period. For the early and late phasesof adaptation and postadaptation, these parameters wereaveraged over the first five strides and last five strides,respectively.

Changes in economy associated with split-beltadaptation. During all phases of the experiment,economy was quantified by estimating the metabolicpower of each participant. Metabolic power was computedbased on the rate of oxygen consumption and carbondioxide production using a standard equation (Brockway,1987). Net metabolic power was computed by subtractingthe average metabolic power during the 5 min standingperiod from the power measured during walking andthen normalizing to body mass. The breath-by-breathmeasurements of net metabolic power were averaged inconsecutive 10 s bins to maintain consistency betweenour kinematic and metabolic measurements.

Our experiment involved abrupt changes in effortas participants switched between each phase of theexperiment and, as a result, there was a transient change innet metabolic power during the early portion of each phase(Fig. 3). We computed the duration of this transient periodfor each of the baseline blocks by finding the time betweenthe start of each block and the point when metabolic powerreached the average power computed during the last 3 minof the block. This duration was 57 ± 10 s across all sub-jects and therefore we omitted the initial 60 s of the netmetabolic power for each time period (i.e. warm-up, slowbaseline, fast baseline, etc) in our group analysis. Partof this transient period stemmed from the transport lagbetween changes in expired gas concentration and thetime when these gases were detected by the sensors. Ourrecording apparatus included a 1.83 m tube extendingfrom the mouthpiece with a volume of approximately1.6 litres and a passive mixing chamber with a volumeof 4.9 litres. At a ventilation rate of 25 l min−1, near whatwas observed during adaptation, approximately 16 s wouldelapse between the time when a breath was expired andwhen it reached the sensor.

After omitting the first minute of expired gas data fromeach phase, we computed the average metabolic power forspecific phases of the experiment as follows. The metabolicpower for warm-up and each baseline phase was computedby averaging the power for the last 2 min of each block.The metabolic power for early adaptation and early post-adaptation was averaged over the second and third minutesof each period because the initial minute of data from theseperiods was omitted. Finally, the metabolic power for lateadaptation and late postadaptation was averaged over thelast 2 min of each period.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1085

Analysis of EMG. Changes in muscle activity duringadaptation were assessed by quantifying the averageintegrated EMG amplitude for each stride. We firstcomputed the maximal rectified EMG for each muscleduring baseline walking at 1.5 m s−1. The EMG timeseries for each subsequent block were then rectified andexpressed as a percentage of the maximal baseline activity(MBA). To quantify changes in muscle activity duringadaptation, we computed the integrated stance and swingphase EMG for each stride and expressed these values asa percentage of the maximal integrated stance and swingphase EMG during baseline walking at 1.5 m s−1 (MIBA).Overall changes in muscle activity during adaptation werecomputed by comparing the stance and swing phaseEMG during the first five strides (early) and last fivestrides (late) of adaptation. We computed the EMGratio for each participant as the ratio of the averageintegrated EMG amplitude in late adaptation to earlyadaptation. An EMG ratio of one would indicate that

there was no change in the average muscle activity duringadaptation.

Statistical analysis. Repeated-measures ANOVA was usedto test for significant differences in kinematic parametersand net metabolic power across all phases of theexperiment (warm-up, slow baseline, fast baseline, earlyadaptation, late adaptation, early postadaptation andlate postadaptation). When significant differences wereobserved, post hoc analyses were performed using Tukey’sHSD test. Student’s one-sample t test was used to test thenull hypothesis that step length symmetry during lateadaptation was not different from zero. We also usedStudent’s one-sample t test to test the hypothesis thatthe stance and swing phase EMG ratio came from adistribution with a mean of one, which would indicatethat there was no change in muscle activity duringthe respective phase of the gait cycle (stance or swing)

0 200 400 600 800

0.5

0.55

0.6

0.65

0.7

Ste

p Le

ngth

(m

)

0 200 400 600 800

0

0.1

Ste

p Le

ngth

Sym

met

ryBA

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

1.4

Ste

p Ti

me

(s)

Fast to Slow

Slow to Fast

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

0.2

Ste

p Ti

me

Sym

met

ry

C D

Strides Strides

0 200 400 600 800 0 200 400 600 800

Strides Strides

Figure 2. Representative example of kinematic parameters recorded for a single participant duringbaseline walking at 0.5 m s−1, adaptation and postadaptation periodsThe first and second dashed vertical lines delineate the transitions from baseline to adaptation and from adaptationto postadaptation, respectively. Data within each period were averaged using a five-step window. A, step lengthfor each limb. During early adaptation, step lengths are markedly different between limbs, but gradually approacha similar value as adaptation progresses. B, the normalized difference in step length between limbs represented asstep length symmetry. The observed changes in step length between limbs are captured by our measure of steplength symmetry, which highlights the asymmetry during early adaptation and the gradual progression towardssymmetry over the course of adaptation. C, step time for the fast and slow limbs. During early adaptation, steptimes are initially similar, but rapidly diverge as adaptation progresses. D, the normalized difference in step timesrepresented as step time symmetry. Here, the divergence in step times during adaptation is evident by the fastincrease in step time symmetry and a significant temporal asymmetry during late adaptation.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1086 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

over the course of adaptation. In addition, we wantedto determine whether changes in kinematics and EMGwere temporally correlated with the observed changes ineconomy. To this end, we computed Pearson’s correlationcoefficients between the following parameters and thenet metabolic power for each participant: step lengthsymmetry; step time symmetry; and integrated EMGamplitude. The first minute of metabolic power datawas omitted from the correlation analysis owing to thetransient change in power when going from baselinewalking to adaptation. We also determined whether themagnitude of the observed reductions in metabolic powerwas correlated with the magnitude of the changes ineach of our kinematic parameters using linear regression.All statistical procedures were performed using Statistica(Statsoft, Tulsa, OK, USA), and significance was set at the5% level.

Results

Kinematic and metabolic changes during split-beltadaptation

The perturbation in belt speeds during adaptationproduced robust changes in step length symmetry andtiming, as well as subsequent effects characteristic of motor

adaptation. An example from a single participant showsstep length symmetry (Fig. 4A) and step time symmetry(Fig. 4B) recorded during all phases of the experiment.During baseline walking, both step length symmetry andstep time symmetry remained near zero for the durationof each period. However, these parameters varied in aninverse manner over the course of adaptation. The changein step length during adaptation was characterized bya large initial asymmetry (approximately 10% of stridelength; Fig. 4A) and a gradual return to a baselinevalue of approximately zero. In contrast, the differencein step times became increasingly asymmetric, plateauingat approximately 15% of the stride time (Fig. 4B). Whenthe belts were driven at the same speed during post-adaptation, a large asymmetry in step length was observedin the opposite direction of the initial asymmetry and wasgradually washed out over the course of postadaptation.Likewise, the difference in step times during post-adaptation was also characterized by an initial asymmetryand gradual decline towards baseline.

Similar changes in step length symmetry and timingwere observed in the group data (Fig. 5A–D). We found asignificant effect of time period on step length symmetry(F6,60 = 45.04, P < 0.001). Post hoc analyses revealedthat step length symmetry during early adaptationwas significantly different from late adaptation (early,

0

2

4

6

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

0 5 10 15 20 25 30 32 370

2

4

6

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

Time (min)

1.0 m/s

PostAdaptationFixed Split

1.5 m/s0.5 m/s

1.0 m/s 1.5 m/s 0.5 m/s

Baseline

20

Figure 3. Breath-by-breath net metabolic power for two representative subjectsThe onset of each block is indicated by vertical dashed lines. Green and red data points are used to indicate theslow and fast baseline periods, respectively. Shaded areas within each block indicate the initial transient period of1 min, which was omitted from the analysis.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1087

−0.12 ± 0.02; and late, −0.002 ± 0.02; P < 0.001), butlate adaptation was not different from zero (t10 = −0.099,P = 0.92). This indicates that the participants returnedto their baseline, symmetric walking pattern by the endof adaptation. Early postadaptation was characterized bya large asymmetry of similar magnitude but oppositedirection to what was observed during early adaptation(0.10 ± 0.02). This asymmetry was gradually washed out,moving towards baseline values at the conclusion of thepostadaptation phase (late postadaptation, 0.02 ± 0.01;and baseline, −0.01 ± 0.007; P = 0.37). We also found asignificant effect of time period on the difference in steptimes as measured by step time symmetry (F6,60 = 44.34,P < 0.001). Step time symmetry gradually increased overthe course of adaptation, and post hoc analyses revealedthat step time symmetry was significantly greater duringlate adaptation than early adaptation (early, 0.06 ± 0.01;and late, 0.15 ± 0.008; P < 0.001). This difference in steptiming diminished rapidly during the first steps of post-adaptation and continued to drop as the participantsde-adapted.

As the participants adapted towards symmetric steplengths, they simultaneously reduced their net metabolicpower despite the fact that the speeds of the treadmill beltsdid not change. An example of the net metabolic powerfor a single participant (Fig. 4C) shows an initial peak of4 W kg−1 during early adaptation and a gradual reductionto slightly more than 2 W kg−1 after 5 min of walking. Forthis individual, metabolic power reduced with a gradualtime course similar to that of step length symmetry and

looked less like step time symmetry, which changed morerapidly early on (Fig. 4A). For the group, metabolic powerwas more strongly correlated with changes in step lengthsymmetry than step time symmetry for a majority (8 of11) of our participants (Table 1).

Overall, the restoration of symmetry during adaptationwas associated with a reduction in metabolic power.We found a significant effect of time period onmetabolic power (F6,60 = 46.71, P < 0.001). Post hocanalyses revealed that the metabolic power duringlate adaptation was significantly lower than duringearly adaptation (early, 3.80 ± 0.31 W kg−1; and late,3.05 ± 0.24 W kg−1; P = 0.003; Fig. 5F). Although theaverage belt speed during adaptation (1 m s−1) wasequal to the speed during the warm-up, the netmetabolic power during late adaptation was significantlygreater than the power measured during warm-up(warm-up, 2.38 ± 0.07 W kg−1; and late adaptation,3.05 ± 0.24 W kg−1; P = 0.01). Therefore, althoughwalking with symmetric step lengths was more economicalthan asymmetric walking, there was an additional powerdemand above what was required for walking with bothbelts moving at the mean speed of 1.0 m s−1.

The overall changes in metabolic power duringadaptation were explained well by the reduction in steplength asymmetry from early to late adaptation. Ourbetween-subjects comparison revealed that the overallreduction in step length asymmetry during adaptationexplained 58% of the variance in the reduction ofmetabolic power (Fig. 6). Subjects who experienced

0.1

0.2

Ste

p Ti

me

Sym

mer

y

0.1

Ste

p Le

ngth

S

ymm

etry

5 10 15 20 25 30 32 37Time (min)

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

A

B

C

Baseline

PostAdaptationAdaptation

20

0

0

0

4

2

1.0 m/s

1.5 m/s0.5 m/s

Figure 4. Step length symmetry (A), steptime symmetry (B) and net metabolic power(C) for a single participantBaseline, adaptation and postadaptation periodsare each denoted at the top of the figure. Eachdata point represents a 10 s average for therespective variable. During adaptation, the timecourse of the decline in net metabolic powerparallels the time course of step length symmetryadaptation.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1088 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

-0.05

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

Ste

p Ti

me

Sym

met

ry-0.1

-0.05

0

0.05

0.1

Ste

p Le

ngth

S

ymm

etry

0 5 10 151

2

3

4

5

Time (min)

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

-0.05

0

0.05

0.1

0.15

-0.1

-0.05

0

0.05

0.1

Ste

p Le

ngth

S

ymm

etry

1.00.5 1.5 EA LA EP LP1

2

3

4

5

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

AdaptationPost Adaptation

*

* *

*

A

FE

DC

B

Ste

p Ti

me

Sym

met

ry

Figure 5. Group data for the adaptation and postadaptation periodsAverage adaptation and post adaptation curves for step length symmetry (A), step time symmetry (C) and netmetabolic power (E). Shaded areas surrounding curves represent standard errors. Vertical boxes at the beginningof adaptation and postadaptation highlight the portion of the metabolic power data that was not included in theanalysis. Average values of step length symmetry (B), step time symmetry (D) and net metabolic power (F) duringall phases of the experiment. The numbers on the x-axis specify the belt speeds for baseline periods. Abbreviations:EA, early adaptation; LA, late adaptation; EP, early postadaptation; and LP, late postadaptation. ∗P < 0.05.

0 0.04 0.08 0.12 0.16

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

1.4

1.6

Reduction in Step Length Asymmetry

Red

uctio

n in

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

r2 = 0.58p = 0.007

0.01 0.03 0.05 0.07

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

1.2

1.4

1.6

Increase in Step Time Asymmetry

Red

uctio

n in

Met

abol

ic P

ower

(W

/kg)

r2 = 0.31p = 0.07

BA

0

More Symmetric Less Symmetric

Figure 6. Reduction in metabolic power versus the change in step length symmetry (A) and step timesymmetry (B) during adaptationPositive values indicate a reduction in metabolic power for both plots. A, positive values on the horizontal axisindicate that step lengths were more symmetric during late adaptation relative to early adaptation. B, positivevalues on the horizontal axis indicate that step times were more asymmetric during late adaptation relative to earlyadaptation. The r2 value and the P value associated with each regression are shown on the figure.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1089

Table 1. Pearson correlation coefficients (r values) betweenstep length symmetry, step time symmetry and net metabolicpower for each participant

Participant Step length Step timenumber symmetry symmetry

P1 0.55 0.41P2 0.50 0.27P3 0.47 0.33P4 0.45 n.s.P5 0.30 0.40P6 0.35 0.45P7 0.52 n.s.P8 0.39 0.25P9 0.52 n.s.P10 0.43 0.43P11 n.s. 0.40Mean ± SEM 0.45 ± 0.02 0.37 ± 0.02

(n) (n = 10) (n = 8)

Parameters that were not correlated with net metabolicpower are denoted as non-significant (n.s.). For the groupdata, the average correlation coefficient, standard error andthe number of participants with significant correlations arepresented in the last two rows of the table.

the largest reductions in asymmetry from early tolate adaptation typically had the largest reductions inmetabolic power and vice versa. In contrast, there was atrend towards a negative relationship between the changein step time symmetry and the reduction in metabolicpower (linear regression, P = 0.07). This suggests thatincreases in step time asymmetry oppose the increase ineconomy associated with reducing step length asymmetry.

It is possible that the overall reduction in metabolicpower could be biased by individuals who walked at thefast speed (1.5 m s−1) immediately prior to adaptation(e.g. Fig. 3A). Our post hoc analysis revealed thata greater net metabolic power was associated withwalking at the fast speed relative to the slow speed(fast, 3.42 ± 0.1 W kg−1; and slow, 1.57 ± 0.07 W kg−1,P < 0.001), which might result in a higher initial powerduring early adaptation. We did not find a significant effectof baseline order on the change in metabolic power fromearly to late adaptation (F1,9 = 2.61, P = 0.14), thoughsubjects who walked at the slower speed immediatelyprior to adaptation tended to have a larger reduction inmetabolic power (fast prior, −0.44 ± 0.23 W kg−1; andslow prior, −0.91 ± 0.18 W kg−1). Importantly, 10 of 11subjects clearly reduced metabolic power over the courseof adaptation.

Changes in muscle activity during adaptation

Changes in muscle activity throughout the gait cycle wereconsistent with the observed reduction in net metabolic

power during adaptation. Average normalized EMG tracesfor a representative participant during early and lateadaptation illustrate this change (Fig. 7). For many ofthe muscles, most notably LG and TA of the fast limband TA of the slow limb, there is a reduction in muscleactivity in late adaptation (grey lines with black shading)relative to early adaptation (black lines with grey shading).This observation was confirmed in the group averagesof the EMG ratio in each muscle (Fig. 8). Here, stancephase muscle activity (Fig. 8A and C) was reduced inall but one of the muscles (BF of the slow limb) duringlate adaptation relative to early adaptation (single-samplet tests, all P < 0.05). The largest reductions in stancephase EMG activity were observed in TA (17 ± 5%) andLG (29 ± 8%) of the fast limb and TA (35 ± 7%) andVL (14 ± 5%) of the slow limb. These reductions didnot appear to result from reduced agonist–antagonistco-contraction over the course of adaptation. During theperiod of peak activity in LG of the fast limb, its antagonist,TA, was minimally active (Fig. 7). Likewise, during theperiod of peak activity in TA of the slow limb, LG wasrelatively quiescent. For the swing phase (Fig. 8B andD), muscle activity was reduced in all muscles duringlate adaptation relative to early adaptation (single-samplet tests, all P < 0.05). The largest reduction in EMG wasobserved in TA of the slow limb (25 ± 9%), while thereduction in other muscles ranged from 8 to 20%.

The time course of changes in EMG amplitude duringadaptation was also consistent with the time course of theobserved reduction in net metabolic power. The integratedEMG amplitude during stance and swing for all muscleswas gradually reduced over the course of adaptation(Fig. 9). We computed correlation coefficients betweenthe time series of EMG amplitude and net metabolicpower for each participant to determine whether thesemeasures were reduced at similar rates (Table 2). Althoughsignificant correlations between muscle activity andmetabolic power were observed for most muscles, thestrongest correlations for both the fast and slow limbswere found for TA (fast, r = 0.50 ± 0.02; and slow,r = 0.48 ± 0.01) and LG (fast, r = 0.45 ± 0.02; and slow,r = 0.53 ± 0.09) The presence of temporal correlationsbetween muscle activity and net metabolic power providesevidence that a gradual reduction in muscle activity islikely to drive the observed reduction in metabolic power.

Discussion

Our results demonstrate that the reduction of steplength asymmetry during split-belt treadmill adaptationis associated with a reduction in the metabolic powerassociated with walking. This reduction in power istemporally correlated with changes in step lengthsymmetry, and the size of this reduction is stronglycorrelated with the magnitude of the improvement in step

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1090 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

TA TA

LG LG

BF

BF

0 20 40 60 80 100

VL

0 20 40 60 80 100

VL

Fast Limb Slow Limb

100% ofMBA

% of Stride% of Stride

Early AdaptationLate Adaptation

Figure 7. Average EMG traces during early and late adaptation for a representative subjectEach trace is the average rectified EMG of five consecutive steps of the fast limb (left side) or slow limb (rightside). For each muscle, EMG amplitude is expressed as a percentage of the peak activity for the respective muscleduring fast baseline walking. Stance phase was normalized to 0–60% of stride time and swing was normalizedto 60–100% of stride time. Horizontal bars at the beginning of each stride indicate a normalized EMG amplitudeof zero. Black lines with grey shading denote early adaptation, while grey lines with black shading denote lateadaptation. Shading represents standard errors. Abbreviations: BF, biceps femoris; LG, lateral gastrocnemius; TA,tibialis anterior; and VL, vastus lateralis.

TA LG BF VL0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Fas

t EM

G R

atio

(Lat

e/E

arly

)

Stance Phase

TA LG BF VL0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Fas

t EM

G R

atio

(Lat

e/E

arly

)

Swing Phase

TA LG BF VL0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Slo

w E

MG

Rat

io(L

ate/

Ear

ly)

TA LG BF VL0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

1

Slo

w E

MG

Rat

io(L

ate/

Ear

ly)

* * ** * * **

* * * * * **

A

DC

B

Stance Phase Swing Phase

Figure 8. Electromyogram ratio for theaverage muscle activity during stanceand swing phasesA, stance phase EMG ratio for the fast limb.B, swing phase EMG ratio for the fast limb.C, stance phase EMG ratio for the slowlimb. D, swing phase EMG ratio for the slowlimb. The horizontal dashed lines representa value of one. Values less than one indicatethat the average muscle activity during lateadaptation was less than the activitymeasured during early adaptation.Abbreviations are as for Fig. 7. Asterisksdenote a significant difference from early tolate adaptation at the P < 0.05 level.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1091

Tab

le2.

Pear

son

corr

elat

ion

coef

fici

ents

(rva

lues

)b

etw

een

inte

gra

ted

EMG

amp

litu

de

(sta

nce

and

swin

g)

and

net

met

abo

licp

ow

er

Part

icip

ant

Fast

leg

Slo

wle

g

nu

mb

erTA

LGB

FV

LTA

LGB

FV

L

P10.

430.

500.

350.

39n.

s.0.

330.

360.

35P2

0.45

0.31

n.s.

0.52

0.51

0.25

0.45

0.43

P30.

540.

560.

350.

430.

480.

380.

280.

42P4

0.50

0.47

0.38

0.37

0.45

0.48

0.46

n.s.

P50.

560.

540.

390.

250.

490.

30n.

s.n.

s.P6

n.s

.0.

36n.

s.n.

s.n.

s.−0

.26

n.s.

−0.2

5P7

0.52

0.47

−0.3

20.

450.

520.

35n.

s.n.

s.P8

0.45

0.45

0.50

n.s.

0.48

n.s.

n.s.

0.33

P90.

560.

42n.

s.0.

420.

48n.

s.0.

290.

48P1

0n

.s.

0.41

n.s.

−0.3

10.

45n.

s.n.

s.n.

s.P1

10.

540.

490.

570.

550.

430.

53n.

s.0.

46M

ean

±SE

M0.

50±

0.02

0.45

±0.

020.

32±

0.11

0.34

±0.

090.

48±

0.01

0.53

±0.

090.

37±

0.04

0.32

±0.

10n

(n=

9)(n

=11

)(n

=6)

(n=

9)(n

=9)

(n=

8)(n

=5)

(n=

7)

Ab

bre

viat

ion

s:B

F,b

icep

sfe

mo

ris;

LG,l

ater

alg

astr

ocn

emiu

s;TA

,tib

ialis

ante

rio

r;an

dV

L,va

stu

sla

tera

lis.M

usc

les

that

wer

en

ot

corr

elat

edw

ith

net

met

abo

licp

ow

erar

ed

eno

ted

asn

on

-sig

nifi

can

t(n

.s.)

.Th

eav

erag

eco

rrel

atio

nco

effi

cien

t,st

and

ard

erro

ran

dth

en

um

ber

of

par

tici

pan

tsw

ith

sig

nifi

can

tco

rrel

atio

ns

are

pre

sen

ted

inth

ela

sttw

oro

ws

of

the

tab

le.

length symmetry. Furthermore, the reduction in poweris accompanied by a simultaneous, bilateral reduction inlower limb muscle activity. Overall, these findings provideevidence that the nervous system is able to optimize ourwalking pattern rapidly to reduce energy expenditure inthe presence of novel dynamic constraints.

Although motor adaptations occur in a variety ofcontexts, the reasons why the nervous system elects torestore certain aspects of movements have yet to beestablished. In the upper limb, there are conflicting viewson the role of energetic optimization during force fieldadaptation. One study demonstrated that individualswere unable to adapt their freely selected hand paths tofollow the minimal energy path as defined by end-pointwork (Kistemaker et al. 2010), but this study did notmeasure metabolic power. In contrast, a more recent studyfound that there are indeed measurable reductions inmetabolic power during force field adaptation (Huanget al. 2012); however, the changes in power occurred overa much longer time scale than the changes in reachingerror and muscle activity. Thus, it appears that motorlearning and energy use are not temporally coupled in thecontext of reaching. In the present study, we not onlydemonstrated that metabolic power is reduced duringlocomotor adaptation, but we have also shown that thetime course of this reduction parallels the time course ofchanges in both step length symmetry and muscle activity.This, along with the correlation between the magnitudeof step length symmetry improvement and the reductionin metabolic power, suggests that it is the systematic,stride-by-stride changes in interlimb co-ordination thatdrive the observed reduction in power. Therefore, itis possible that an energy-minimization process drivessplit-belt locomotor adaptation.

Energy reduction during motor learning of novel skillshas been demonstrated previously (Childs et al. 2002;Lay et al. 2002; Galna & Sparrow, 2006). However, unlikeshort-term motor adaptation, skill learning often involveslong-term practice over multiple days and, once the skillis learned, it can immediately be used in the appropriatecontext (Bastian, 2008). Reductions in metabolic powerand muscle activity have also been observed over multipledays of adaptation to an assistive torque provided bya powered ankle exoskeleton (Sawicki & Ferris, 2008).Although changes in metabolic power over multiple dayshave been associated with reductions in muscle activity(Lay et al. 2002; Sawicki & Ferris, 2008), the long time scalenecessary to observe these changes would not predict thatmetabolic power would be reduced during a single sessionof motor adaptation.

Kinematic contributions to metabolic powerreduction

Although step length symmetry is used as our measure oferror during adaptation, it is not clear whether symmetry

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1092 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

itself or other kinematic changes drive the reductionin metabolic power. Split-belt adaptation involves bothspatial and temporal changes in the walking pattern(Malone & Bastian, 2010), and each of these is likelyto influence the economy of walking. The step lengthasymmetry present during early adaptation is graduallyreduced by shortening the slow step and lengtheningthe fast step. Collisional models and experimentaldata demonstrate that metabolic power should increasenon-linearly as steps are lengthened and decrease as stepsare shortened (Donelan et al. 2002; Kuo, 2002). Thus,the observed changes in step length are likely to opposeone another, and the change in net metabolic powerwill depend on the relative magnitude of the individualchanges in step length.

Likewise, metabolic power scales non-linearly withthe frequency of leg swing (Doke et al. 2005), whichis inversely proportional to step time. During earlyadaptation, the step times diverge, with the slow step timeincreasing and the fast step time decreasing. This trendcontinues throughout adaptation, leading to opposingeffects on the net metabolic power. Like step length,the change in metabolic power due to changes in steptime will depend on the difference in magnitude of thesechanges between limbs. Given these non-linear inter-actions between step length and step time, further researchis necessary to quantify the contributions of spatialand temporal adjustments to economy during split-beltadaptation.

Ultimately, the observed reduction in metabolic poweris likely to be driven by muscle contraction, and our EMGdata provide insight into which muscles may be drivingthis change in power. Over the course of adaptation, the

average integrated muscle activity was reduced in nearlyall of the muscles that we investigated. This reductionwas most evident during late stance for the lateral gastro-cnemius of the fast limb and during early stance for tibialisanterior of the slow limb. It was previously demonstratedthat these muscles were more active during split-beltwalking relative to walking with the belts moving at thesame speeds (Dietz et al. 1994), but the authors didnot indicate whether this activity changed as participantsadapted to walking on the treadmill.

There are multiple factors which may contribute to thechange in muscle activity throughout adaptation. Duringearly adaptation, the leg on the fast belt is extendedmore rapidly and further than expected, resulting intwo potential consequences. First, this could cause alarge impact at slow heel strike, potentially resulting instretch-induced activity in the slow TA during early stance.This collision might also require a higher level of ankleextension torque to restore the energy lost during thestep-to-step transition (Kuo, 2001; Donelan et al. 2002).As the slow step length is reduced, the impact at slow heelstrike would gradually be reduced and the fast leg wouldbe in a more optimal, less extended position at preswing,thereby requiring less ankle extension torque. On the slowlimb, the high level of TA activity could be interpreted asan attempt to stabilize the ankle joint during the singlesupport phase. Although we did not observe heightenedco-contraction of the gastrocnemius during this period,it is possible that other muscles which we did not record,such as soleus, were recruited to maintain stability duringearly adaptation. As the participants learned the taskdynamics, this stabilization strategy may have becomeunnecessary.

Fas

t TA

Fas

t LG

Fas

t BF

0 5 10Time (min)

Fas

t VL

Slo

w T

AS

low

LG

Slo

w B

F

0 5 10Time (min)

Slo

w V

L

200 % ofMIBA

100 % ofMIBA

Figure 9. Average adaptation curves for theintegrated EMG amplitude during eachstrideEach curve represents the group average, withstandard errors indicated by shading. For eachmuscle, the EMG amplitude is expressed as apercentage of the maximal EMG integrated overa full stride for the respective muscle during fastbaseline walking (MIBA). The vertical scale baradjacent to the slow TA is for this muscle only;for all other muscles, the scale bar is adjacent tothe slow VL. Horizontal bars at the beginning ofeach trace indicate a normalized EMG amplitudeof 80%. Abbreviations are as for Fig. 7.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1093

Reducing metabolic power: a goal or a byproduct ofadaptation?

It remains to be determined whether the economy ofwalking is directly optimized during split-belt adaptationor if the observed reduction in metabolic power is anindirect consequence of another, possibly error-driven,optimization process. When individuals are initiallyexposed to walking on a split-belt treadmill, it is notobligatory that people would desire to or be able todirectly to optimize the economy of walking. The onlyclear requirement of the task is to select a pattern ofco-ordination that keeps the body upright while perhapsproviding some margin of safety. Outside of this goal,people could pursue any combination of additional goals.For example, it has been proposed that adjustments torapid changes in treadmill speed include a fast, pre-programmed response driven by prior experience anda slower, more gradual change, potentially involvingdirect optimization of energy expenditure (Snaterseet al. 2011). For split-belt adaptation, it is possiblethat the rapid changes associated with step timingand the gradual changes in step lengths reflect similarmechanisms. Alternatively, error-driven processes basedon step length asymmetry or other kinematic parametersmay inherently result in a more economical walkingpattern.

Split-belt adaptation, like other forms of error-basedmotor learning, is a cerebellum-dependent process. Thecerebellum is critically important for practice-dependent,predictive adjustment of movements (Lang & Bastian,2002; Maschke et al. 2004; Smith & Shadmehr, 2005),and damage to the cerebellum impairs split-belt treadmilladaptation (Morton & Bastian, 2006). However, the roleof the cerebellum in reducing energy expenditure duringadaptation has yet to be established. One possibility is thatthe cerebellum is only involved in adjusting for kinematicprediction errors and the observed reduction in energyexpenditure is simply a byproduct of this process. Otherpossibilities require the cerebellum to have access to someestimate of effort or energy expenditure, which could beprovided using a combination of somatosensory feed-back (Sanes & Shadmehr, 1995), afferent feedback fromchemoreceptors (Heymans, 1963; Gestreau et al. 2010)or an efference copy of motor commands (Miall et al.1993). Thus, the cerebellum could be used to minimizethe error between the expected effort needed to walkat the perceived speed and the effort sensed throughcentral and peripheral signals. Lastly, the cerebellummay be involved in direct minimization of energyexpenditure by developing a systematic representationof the relationship between changes in motorcommands and estimated changes in effort or energyexpenditure.

Broader implications for the recovery of locomotorfunction

One issue that has yet to be addressed is how ourfindings relate to the processes that drive locomotorrecovery following injury or disease. For example, walkingasymmetry is commonly observed after physical injury(e.g. amputation) or neurological injury (e.g. unilateralstroke). Over the course of recovery, it is possible thatthese patients adapt to the loss of biomechanical and/orneural control options and adopt the most economical gaitpattern given these constraints. Alternatively, the damageto the nervous system resulting from stroke could disruptthe circuits responsible for energetic optimization, therebyresulting in a suboptimal gait.

For both amputees and stroke survivors, it is alsoconceivable that the goal of economy is superseded byconstraints on maintaining a stable gait that reducesthe likelihood of falling. Additional studies are necessaryto determine how these factors influence the recoveryprocess.

The relationship between symmetry and economyfollowing stroke remains an open question that maybe important for guiding the rehabilitation process.Stroke survivors are capable of improving symmetrythrough training on a split-belt treadmill (Reisman etal. 2007, 2009), and these changes can be maintainedwith repeated exposures to the treadmill (Reisman et al.2010). What remains to be seen is whether symmetryimproves economy in these individuals. This is importantbecause if an abnormally high rate of energy expenditurelimits the self-selected walking speed for stroke survivors,then an increase in economy may promote fasterwalking.

References

Alexander RM (1997). A minimum energy cost hypothesis forhuman arm trajectories. Biol Cybern 76, 97–105.

Alexander RM (2002). Principles of Animal Locomotion.Princeton University Press, Princeton.

Bastian AJ (2008). Understanding sensorimotor adaptation andlearning for rehabilitation. Curr Opin Neurol 21, 628–633.

Bertram JE & Ruina A (2001). Multiple walkingspeed-frequency relations are predicted by constrainedoptimization. J Theor Biol 209, 445–453.

Brockway JM (1987). Derivation of formulae used to calculateenergy expenditure in man. Hum Nutr Clin Nutr 41,463–471.

Childs JD, Gantt C, Higgins D, Papazis JA, Franklin R, MetzlerT & Underwood FB (2002). The effect of repeated bouts ofbackward walking on physiologic efficiency. J Strength CondRes 16, 451–455.

Choi JT, Vining EP, Reisman DS & Bastian AJ (2009). Walkingflexibility after hemispherectomy: split-belt treadmilladaptation and feedback control. Brain 132, 722–733.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

1094 J. M. Finley and others J Physiol 591.4

Collins SH, Adamczyk PG, Ferris DP & Kuo AD (2009). Asimple method for calibrating force plates and forcetreadmills using an instrumented pole. Gait Posture 29,59–64.

Cram JR & Kasman GS (1998). Electrode Placement .Gaithersburg, Aspen.

Dietz V, Zijlstra W & Duysens J (1994). Human neuronalinterlimb coordination during split-belt locomotion. ExpBrain Res 101, 513–520.

Doke J, Donelan JM & Kuo AD (2005). Mechanics andenergetics of swinging the human leg. J Exp Biol 208,439–445.

Donelan JM, Kram R & Kuo AD (2001). Mechanical andmetabolic determinants of the preferred step width inhuman walking. Proc Biol Sci 268, 1985–1992.

Donelan JM, Kram R & Kuo AD (2002). Mechanical work forstep-to-step transitions is a major determinant of themetabolic cost of human walking. J Exp Biol 205, 3717–3727.

Emken JL, Benitez R, Sideris A, Bobrow JE & ReinkensmeyerDJ (2007). Motor adaptation as a greedy optimization oferror and effort. J Neurophysiol 97, 3997–4006.

Emken JL & Reinkensmeyer DJ (2005). Robot-enhanced motorlearning: accelerating internal model formation duringlocomotion by transient dynamic amplification. IEEE TransNeural Syst Rehabil Eng 13, 33–39.

Galna B & Sparrow WA (2006). Learning to minimize energycosts and maximize mechanical work in a bimanualcoordination task. J Mot Behav 38, 411–422.

Gestreau C, Heitzmann D, Thomas J, Dubreuil V, Bandulik S,Reichold M, Bendahhou S, Pierson P, Sterner C,Peyronnet-Roux J, Benfriha C, Tegtmeier I, Ehnes H,Georgieff M, Lesage F, Brunet J-F, Goridis C, Warth R &Barhanin J (2010). Task2 potassium channels set centralrespiratory CO2 and O2 sensitivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A107, 2325–2330.

Hatze H & Buys JD (1977). Energy-optimal controls in themammalian neuromuscular system. Biol Cybern 27, 9–20.

Heymans C (1963). A look at an old but still current problem.Annu Rev Physiol 25, 1–16.

Huang HJ, Kram R & Ahmed AA (2012). Reduction ofmetabolic cost during motor learning of arm reachingdynamics. J Neurosci 32, 2182–2190.

Kistemaker DA, Wong JD & Gribble PL (2010). The centralnervous system does not minimize energy cost in armmovements. J Neurophysiol 104, 2985–2994.

Kram R, Griffin TM, Donelan JM & Chang YH (1998). Forcetreadmill for measuring vertical and horizontal groundreaction forces. J Appl Physiol 85, 764–769.

Kuo AD (2001). A simple model of bipedal walking predicts thepreferred speed-step length relationship. J Biomech Eng 123,264–269.

Kuo AD (2002). Energetics of actively powered locomotionusing the simplest walking model. J Biomech Eng 124,113–120.

Lam T, Anderschitz M, Dietz V (2006). Contribution offeedback and feedforward strategies to locomotoradaptations. J Neurophysiol 95, 766–773.

Lang CE & Bastian AJ (2002). Cerebellar damage impairsautomaticity of a recently practiced movement. JNeurophysiol 87, 1336–1347.

Lay B, Sparrow W, Hughes K & O’Dwyer N (2002). Practiceeffects on coordination and control, metabolic energyexpenditure, and muscle activation. Hum Mov Sci 21,807–830.

Malone LA & Bastian AJ (2010). Thinking about walking:Effects of conscious correction versus distraction onlocomotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol 103, 1954–1962.

Malone LA, Bastian AJ & Torres-Oviedo G (2012). How doesthe motor system correct for errors in time and space duringlocomotor adaptation? J Neurophysiol 108, 672–683.

Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ & Thach WT(1996). Throwing while looking through prisms. II.Specificity and storage of multiple gaze–throw calibrations.Brain 119, 1199–1211.

Maschke M, Gomez CM, Ebner TJ & Konczak J (2004).Hereditary cerebellar ataxia progressively impairs forceadaptation during goal-directed arm movements. JNeurophysiol 91, 230–238.

Miall RC, Weir DJ, Wolpert DM & Stein JF (1993). Is thecerebellum a Smith Predictor? J Mot Behav 25, 203–216.

Morton SM & Bastian AJ (2006). Cerebellar contributions tolocomotor adaptations during splitbelt treadmill walking. JNeurosci 26, 9107–9116.

Reisman DS, Block HJ & Bastian AJ (2005). Interlimbcoordination during locomotion: what can be adapted andstored? J Neurophysiol 94, 2403–2415.

Reisman DS, McLean H & Bastian AJ (2010). Split-belttreadmill training poststroke: a case study. J Neurol Phys Ther34, 202–207.

Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K & Bastian AJ (2007). Locomotoradaptation on a split-belt treadmill can improve walkingsymmetry post-stroke. Brain 130, 1861–1872.

Reisman DS, Wityk R, Silver K & Bastian AJ (2009). Split-belttreadmill adaptation transfers to overground walking inpersons poststroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 23, 735–744.

Sanes JN & Shadmehr R (1995). Sense of muscular effort andsomesthetic afferent information in humans. Can J PhysiolPharmacol 73, 223–233.

Sawicki GS & Ferris DP (2008). Mechanics and energetics oflevel walking with powered ankle exoskeletons. J Exp Biol211, 1402–1413.

Smith MA & Shadmehr R (2005). Intact ability to learn internalmodels of arm dynamics in Huntington’s disease but notcerebellar degeneration. J Neurophysiol 93, 2809–2821.

Snaterse M, Ton R, Kuo AD & Donelan JM (2011). Distinct fastand slow processes contribute to the selection of preferredstep frequency during human walking. J Appl Physiol 110,1682–1690.

Todorov E (2004). Optimality principles in sensorimotorcontrol. Nat Neurosci 7, 907–915.

Umberger BR & Martin PE (2007). Mechanical power andefficiency of level walking with different stride rates. J ExpBiol 210, 3255–3265.

Waters RL & Mulroy S (1999). The energy expenditure ofnormal and pathologic gait. Gait Posture 9, 207–231.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society

J Physiol 591.4 Split-belt adaptation reduces metabolic power 1095

Winter DA, Fuglevand AJ & Archer SE (1994). Crosstalk insurface electromyography: theoretical and practicalestimates. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 4, 15–26.

Zarrugh MY & Radcliffe CW (1978). Predicting metabolic costof level walking. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol 38,215–223.

Zarrugh MY, Todd FN & Ralston HJ (1974). Optimization ofenergy expenditure during level walking. Eur J Appl Physiol33, 293–306.

Author contributions

All experiments were performed in the laboratory of J.S.G.All authors contributed to the conception and design of the

study. J.M.F. and J.S.G. collected data for the study. All authorscontributed to the analysis and interpretation of the data and thedrafting of the manuscript. The final version of the manuscriptwas approved by all authors.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by NIH grants HD048741 andHD007414. The authors would like to thank Eileen Barno forassisting with data collection and Max Donelan for providingfeedback on an earlier draft of the manuscript.

C© 2013 The Authors. The Journal of Physiology C© 2013 The Physiological Society


Recommended